Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech
Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book...
Ausführliche Beschreibung
Autor*in: |
Alvin Wei Ming Tan [verfasserIn] Kate Nation [verfasserIn] Nicola Dawson [verfasserIn] Nilanjana Banerji [verfasserIn] Yaling Hsiao [verfasserIn] |
---|
Format: |
E-Artikel |
---|---|
Sprache: |
Englisch |
Erschienen: |
2021 |
---|
Schlagwörter: |
---|
Übergeordnetes Werk: |
In: Language Development Research - Carnegie Mellon University Library Publishing Service, 2024, 1(2021), 1 |
---|---|
Übergeordnetes Werk: |
volume:1 ; year:2021 ; number:1 |
Links: |
Link aufrufen |
---|
DOI / URN: |
10.34842/5we1-yk94 |
---|
Katalog-ID: |
DOAJ100834086 |
---|
LEADER | 01000naa a22002652 4500 | ||
---|---|---|---|
001 | DOAJ100834086 | ||
003 | DE-627 | ||
005 | 20240414130229.0 | ||
007 | cr uuu---uuuuu | ||
008 | 240414s2021 xx |||||o 00| ||eng c | ||
024 | 7 | |a 10.34842/5we1-yk94 |2 doi | |
035 | |a (DE-627)DOAJ100834086 | ||
035 | |a (DE-599)DOAJ78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 | ||
040 | |a DE-627 |b ger |c DE-627 |e rakwb | ||
041 | |a eng | ||
050 | 0 | |a P1-1091 | |
100 | 0 | |a Alvin Wei Ming Tan |e verfasserin |4 aut | |
245 | 1 | 0 | |a Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech |
264 | 1 | |c 2021 | |
336 | |a Text |b txt |2 rdacontent | ||
337 | |a Computermedien |b c |2 rdamedia | ||
338 | |a Online-Ressource |b cr |2 rdacarrier | ||
520 | |a Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book corpus from 160 texts commonly read to children aged 0-5 years (around 320,000 words). We first quantified how the language of children’s books differs from child-directed speech (compiled from 10 corpora in the CHILDES UK database, around 3.8 million words) on measures of lexical richness (diversity, density, sophistication), part of speech distributions, and structural properties. We also identified the words occurring in children’s books that are most uniquely representative of book language. We found that children’s book language is lexically denser, more lexically diverse, and comprises a larger proportion of rarer word types compared to child-directed speech. Nouns and adjectives are more common in book language whereas pronouns are more common in child-directed speech. Book words are more structurally complex in relation to both number of phonemes and morphological structure. They are also later acquired, more abstract, and more emotionally arousing than the words more common in child-directed speech. Written language provides unique linguistic input even in the pre-school years, well before children can read for themselves. | ||
650 | 4 | |a lexical richness | |
650 | 4 | |a book language | |
650 | 4 | |a children | |
650 | 4 | |a child-directed speech | |
650 | 4 | |a language acquisition | |
650 | 4 | |a literacy | |
653 | 0 | |a Philology. Linguistics | |
700 | 0 | |a Kate Nation |e verfasserin |4 aut | |
700 | 0 | |a Nicola Dawson |e verfasserin |4 aut | |
700 | 0 | |a Nilanjana Banerji |e verfasserin |4 aut | |
700 | 0 | |a Yaling Hsiao |e verfasserin |4 aut | |
773 | 0 | 8 | |i In |t Language Development Research |d Carnegie Mellon University Library Publishing Service, 2024 |g 1(2021), 1 |w (DE-627)DOAJ090663721 |x 27717976 |7 nnns |
773 | 1 | 8 | |g volume:1 |g year:2021 |g number:1 |
856 | 4 | 0 | |u https://doi.org/10.34842/5we1-yk94 |z kostenfrei |
856 | 4 | 0 | |u https://doaj.org/article/78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 |z kostenfrei |
856 | 4 | 0 | |u https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/id/77/ |z kostenfrei |
856 | 4 | 0 | |u https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/77/galley/451/download/ |z kostenfrei |
856 | 4 | 2 | |u https://doaj.org/toc/2771-7976 |y Journal toc |z kostenfrei |
912 | |a GBV_USEFLAG_A | ||
912 | |a SYSFLAG_A | ||
912 | |a GBV_DOAJ | ||
951 | |a AR | ||
952 | |d 1 |j 2021 |e 1 |
author_variant |
a w m t awmt k n kn n d nd n b nb y h yh |
---|---|
matchkey_str |
article:27717976:2021----::etrsfeiarcnsicidesokcmaiosi |
hierarchy_sort_str |
2021 |
callnumber-subject-code |
P |
publishDate |
2021 |
allfields |
10.34842/5we1-yk94 doi (DE-627)DOAJ100834086 (DE-599)DOAJ78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 DE-627 ger DE-627 rakwb eng P1-1091 Alvin Wei Ming Tan verfasserin aut Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech 2021 Text txt rdacontent Computermedien c rdamedia Online-Ressource cr rdacarrier Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book corpus from 160 texts commonly read to children aged 0-5 years (around 320,000 words). We first quantified how the language of children’s books differs from child-directed speech (compiled from 10 corpora in the CHILDES UK database, around 3.8 million words) on measures of lexical richness (diversity, density, sophistication), part of speech distributions, and structural properties. We also identified the words occurring in children’s books that are most uniquely representative of book language. We found that children’s book language is lexically denser, more lexically diverse, and comprises a larger proportion of rarer word types compared to child-directed speech. Nouns and adjectives are more common in book language whereas pronouns are more common in child-directed speech. Book words are more structurally complex in relation to both number of phonemes and morphological structure. They are also later acquired, more abstract, and more emotionally arousing than the words more common in child-directed speech. Written language provides unique linguistic input even in the pre-school years, well before children can read for themselves. lexical richness book language children child-directed speech language acquisition literacy Philology. Linguistics Kate Nation verfasserin aut Nicola Dawson verfasserin aut Nilanjana Banerji verfasserin aut Yaling Hsiao verfasserin aut In Language Development Research Carnegie Mellon University Library Publishing Service, 2024 1(2021), 1 (DE-627)DOAJ090663721 27717976 nnns volume:1 year:2021 number:1 https://doi.org/10.34842/5we1-yk94 kostenfrei https://doaj.org/article/78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 kostenfrei https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/id/77/ kostenfrei https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/77/galley/451/download/ kostenfrei https://doaj.org/toc/2771-7976 Journal toc kostenfrei GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_DOAJ AR 1 2021 1 |
spelling |
10.34842/5we1-yk94 doi (DE-627)DOAJ100834086 (DE-599)DOAJ78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 DE-627 ger DE-627 rakwb eng P1-1091 Alvin Wei Ming Tan verfasserin aut Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech 2021 Text txt rdacontent Computermedien c rdamedia Online-Ressource cr rdacarrier Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book corpus from 160 texts commonly read to children aged 0-5 years (around 320,000 words). We first quantified how the language of children’s books differs from child-directed speech (compiled from 10 corpora in the CHILDES UK database, around 3.8 million words) on measures of lexical richness (diversity, density, sophistication), part of speech distributions, and structural properties. We also identified the words occurring in children’s books that are most uniquely representative of book language. We found that children’s book language is lexically denser, more lexically diverse, and comprises a larger proportion of rarer word types compared to child-directed speech. Nouns and adjectives are more common in book language whereas pronouns are more common in child-directed speech. Book words are more structurally complex in relation to both number of phonemes and morphological structure. They are also later acquired, more abstract, and more emotionally arousing than the words more common in child-directed speech. Written language provides unique linguistic input even in the pre-school years, well before children can read for themselves. lexical richness book language children child-directed speech language acquisition literacy Philology. Linguistics Kate Nation verfasserin aut Nicola Dawson verfasserin aut Nilanjana Banerji verfasserin aut Yaling Hsiao verfasserin aut In Language Development Research Carnegie Mellon University Library Publishing Service, 2024 1(2021), 1 (DE-627)DOAJ090663721 27717976 nnns volume:1 year:2021 number:1 https://doi.org/10.34842/5we1-yk94 kostenfrei https://doaj.org/article/78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 kostenfrei https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/id/77/ kostenfrei https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/77/galley/451/download/ kostenfrei https://doaj.org/toc/2771-7976 Journal toc kostenfrei GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_DOAJ AR 1 2021 1 |
allfields_unstemmed |
10.34842/5we1-yk94 doi (DE-627)DOAJ100834086 (DE-599)DOAJ78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 DE-627 ger DE-627 rakwb eng P1-1091 Alvin Wei Ming Tan verfasserin aut Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech 2021 Text txt rdacontent Computermedien c rdamedia Online-Ressource cr rdacarrier Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book corpus from 160 texts commonly read to children aged 0-5 years (around 320,000 words). We first quantified how the language of children’s books differs from child-directed speech (compiled from 10 corpora in the CHILDES UK database, around 3.8 million words) on measures of lexical richness (diversity, density, sophistication), part of speech distributions, and structural properties. We also identified the words occurring in children’s books that are most uniquely representative of book language. We found that children’s book language is lexically denser, more lexically diverse, and comprises a larger proportion of rarer word types compared to child-directed speech. Nouns and adjectives are more common in book language whereas pronouns are more common in child-directed speech. Book words are more structurally complex in relation to both number of phonemes and morphological structure. They are also later acquired, more abstract, and more emotionally arousing than the words more common in child-directed speech. Written language provides unique linguistic input even in the pre-school years, well before children can read for themselves. lexical richness book language children child-directed speech language acquisition literacy Philology. Linguistics Kate Nation verfasserin aut Nicola Dawson verfasserin aut Nilanjana Banerji verfasserin aut Yaling Hsiao verfasserin aut In Language Development Research Carnegie Mellon University Library Publishing Service, 2024 1(2021), 1 (DE-627)DOAJ090663721 27717976 nnns volume:1 year:2021 number:1 https://doi.org/10.34842/5we1-yk94 kostenfrei https://doaj.org/article/78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 kostenfrei https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/id/77/ kostenfrei https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/77/galley/451/download/ kostenfrei https://doaj.org/toc/2771-7976 Journal toc kostenfrei GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_DOAJ AR 1 2021 1 |
allfieldsGer |
10.34842/5we1-yk94 doi (DE-627)DOAJ100834086 (DE-599)DOAJ78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 DE-627 ger DE-627 rakwb eng P1-1091 Alvin Wei Ming Tan verfasserin aut Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech 2021 Text txt rdacontent Computermedien c rdamedia Online-Ressource cr rdacarrier Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book corpus from 160 texts commonly read to children aged 0-5 years (around 320,000 words). We first quantified how the language of children’s books differs from child-directed speech (compiled from 10 corpora in the CHILDES UK database, around 3.8 million words) on measures of lexical richness (diversity, density, sophistication), part of speech distributions, and structural properties. We also identified the words occurring in children’s books that are most uniquely representative of book language. We found that children’s book language is lexically denser, more lexically diverse, and comprises a larger proportion of rarer word types compared to child-directed speech. Nouns and adjectives are more common in book language whereas pronouns are more common in child-directed speech. Book words are more structurally complex in relation to both number of phonemes and morphological structure. They are also later acquired, more abstract, and more emotionally arousing than the words more common in child-directed speech. Written language provides unique linguistic input even in the pre-school years, well before children can read for themselves. lexical richness book language children child-directed speech language acquisition literacy Philology. Linguistics Kate Nation verfasserin aut Nicola Dawson verfasserin aut Nilanjana Banerji verfasserin aut Yaling Hsiao verfasserin aut In Language Development Research Carnegie Mellon University Library Publishing Service, 2024 1(2021), 1 (DE-627)DOAJ090663721 27717976 nnns volume:1 year:2021 number:1 https://doi.org/10.34842/5we1-yk94 kostenfrei https://doaj.org/article/78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 kostenfrei https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/id/77/ kostenfrei https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/77/galley/451/download/ kostenfrei https://doaj.org/toc/2771-7976 Journal toc kostenfrei GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_DOAJ AR 1 2021 1 |
allfieldsSound |
10.34842/5we1-yk94 doi (DE-627)DOAJ100834086 (DE-599)DOAJ78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 DE-627 ger DE-627 rakwb eng P1-1091 Alvin Wei Ming Tan verfasserin aut Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech 2021 Text txt rdacontent Computermedien c rdamedia Online-Ressource cr rdacarrier Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book corpus from 160 texts commonly read to children aged 0-5 years (around 320,000 words). We first quantified how the language of children’s books differs from child-directed speech (compiled from 10 corpora in the CHILDES UK database, around 3.8 million words) on measures of lexical richness (diversity, density, sophistication), part of speech distributions, and structural properties. We also identified the words occurring in children’s books that are most uniquely representative of book language. We found that children’s book language is lexically denser, more lexically diverse, and comprises a larger proportion of rarer word types compared to child-directed speech. Nouns and adjectives are more common in book language whereas pronouns are more common in child-directed speech. Book words are more structurally complex in relation to both number of phonemes and morphological structure. They are also later acquired, more abstract, and more emotionally arousing than the words more common in child-directed speech. Written language provides unique linguistic input even in the pre-school years, well before children can read for themselves. lexical richness book language children child-directed speech language acquisition literacy Philology. Linguistics Kate Nation verfasserin aut Nicola Dawson verfasserin aut Nilanjana Banerji verfasserin aut Yaling Hsiao verfasserin aut In Language Development Research Carnegie Mellon University Library Publishing Service, 2024 1(2021), 1 (DE-627)DOAJ090663721 27717976 nnns volume:1 year:2021 number:1 https://doi.org/10.34842/5we1-yk94 kostenfrei https://doaj.org/article/78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 kostenfrei https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/id/77/ kostenfrei https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/77/galley/451/download/ kostenfrei https://doaj.org/toc/2771-7976 Journal toc kostenfrei GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_DOAJ AR 1 2021 1 |
language |
English |
source |
In Language Development Research 1(2021), 1 volume:1 year:2021 number:1 |
sourceStr |
In Language Development Research 1(2021), 1 volume:1 year:2021 number:1 |
format_phy_str_mv |
Article |
institution |
findex.gbv.de |
topic_facet |
lexical richness book language children child-directed speech language acquisition literacy Philology. Linguistics |
isfreeaccess_bool |
true |
container_title |
Language Development Research |
authorswithroles_txt_mv |
Alvin Wei Ming Tan @@aut@@ Kate Nation @@aut@@ Nicola Dawson @@aut@@ Nilanjana Banerji @@aut@@ Yaling Hsiao @@aut@@ |
publishDateDaySort_date |
2021-01-01T00:00:00Z |
hierarchy_top_id |
DOAJ090663721 |
id |
DOAJ100834086 |
language_de |
englisch |
fullrecord |
<?xml version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8"?><collection xmlns="http://www.loc.gov/MARC21/slim"><record><leader>01000naa a22002652 4500</leader><controlfield tag="001">DOAJ100834086</controlfield><controlfield tag="003">DE-627</controlfield><controlfield tag="005">20240414130229.0</controlfield><controlfield tag="007">cr uuu---uuuuu</controlfield><controlfield tag="008">240414s2021 xx |||||o 00| ||eng c</controlfield><datafield tag="024" ind1="7" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">10.34842/5we1-yk94</subfield><subfield code="2">doi</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="035" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">(DE-627)DOAJ100834086</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="035" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">(DE-599)DOAJ78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="040" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">DE-627</subfield><subfield code="b">ger</subfield><subfield code="c">DE-627</subfield><subfield code="e">rakwb</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="041" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">eng</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="050" ind1=" " ind2="0"><subfield code="a">P1-1091</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="100" ind1="0" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Alvin Wei Ming Tan</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="245" ind1="1" ind2="0"><subfield code="a">Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="264" ind1=" " ind2="1"><subfield code="c">2021</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="336" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Text</subfield><subfield code="b">txt</subfield><subfield code="2">rdacontent</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="337" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Computermedien</subfield><subfield code="b">c</subfield><subfield code="2">rdamedia</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="338" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Online-Ressource</subfield><subfield code="b">cr</subfield><subfield code="2">rdacarrier</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="520" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book corpus from 160 texts commonly read to children aged 0-5 years (around 320,000 words). We first quantified how the language of children’s books differs from child-directed speech (compiled from 10 corpora in the CHILDES UK database, around 3.8 million words) on measures of lexical richness (diversity, density, sophistication), part of speech distributions, and structural properties. We also identified the words occurring in children’s books that are most uniquely representative of book language. We found that children’s book language is lexically denser, more lexically diverse, and comprises a larger proportion of rarer word types compared to child-directed speech. Nouns and adjectives are more common in book language whereas pronouns are more common in child-directed speech. Book words are more structurally complex in relation to both number of phonemes and morphological structure. They are also later acquired, more abstract, and more emotionally arousing than the words more common in child-directed speech. Written language provides unique linguistic input even in the pre-school years, well before children can read for themselves.</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">lexical richness</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">book language</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">children</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">child-directed speech</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">language acquisition</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">literacy</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="653" ind1=" " ind2="0"><subfield code="a">Philology. Linguistics</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="700" ind1="0" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Kate Nation</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="700" ind1="0" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Nicola Dawson</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="700" ind1="0" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Nilanjana Banerji</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="700" ind1="0" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Yaling Hsiao</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="773" ind1="0" ind2="8"><subfield code="i">In</subfield><subfield code="t">Language Development Research</subfield><subfield code="d">Carnegie Mellon University Library Publishing Service, 2024</subfield><subfield code="g">1(2021), 1</subfield><subfield code="w">(DE-627)DOAJ090663721</subfield><subfield code="x">27717976</subfield><subfield code="7">nnns</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="773" ind1="1" ind2="8"><subfield code="g">volume:1</subfield><subfield code="g">year:2021</subfield><subfield code="g">number:1</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="0"><subfield code="u">https://doi.org/10.34842/5we1-yk94</subfield><subfield code="z">kostenfrei</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="0"><subfield code="u">https://doaj.org/article/78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869</subfield><subfield code="z">kostenfrei</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="0"><subfield code="u">https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/id/77/</subfield><subfield code="z">kostenfrei</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="0"><subfield code="u">https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/77/galley/451/download/</subfield><subfield code="z">kostenfrei</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="2"><subfield code="u">https://doaj.org/toc/2771-7976</subfield><subfield code="y">Journal toc</subfield><subfield code="z">kostenfrei</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_USEFLAG_A</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">SYSFLAG_A</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_DOAJ</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="951" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">AR</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="952" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="d">1</subfield><subfield code="j">2021</subfield><subfield code="e">1</subfield></datafield></record></collection>
|
callnumber-first |
P - Language and Literature |
author |
Alvin Wei Ming Tan |
spellingShingle |
Alvin Wei Ming Tan misc P1-1091 misc lexical richness misc book language misc children misc child-directed speech misc language acquisition misc literacy misc Philology. Linguistics Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech |
authorStr |
Alvin Wei Ming Tan |
ppnlink_with_tag_str_mv |
@@773@@(DE-627)DOAJ090663721 |
format |
electronic Article |
delete_txt_mv |
keep |
author_role |
aut aut aut aut aut |
collection |
DOAJ |
remote_str |
true |
callnumber-label |
P1-1091 |
illustrated |
Not Illustrated |
issn |
27717976 |
topic_title |
P1-1091 Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech lexical richness book language children child-directed speech language acquisition literacy |
topic |
misc P1-1091 misc lexical richness misc book language misc children misc child-directed speech misc language acquisition misc literacy misc Philology. Linguistics |
topic_unstemmed |
misc P1-1091 misc lexical richness misc book language misc children misc child-directed speech misc language acquisition misc literacy misc Philology. Linguistics |
topic_browse |
misc P1-1091 misc lexical richness misc book language misc children misc child-directed speech misc language acquisition misc literacy misc Philology. Linguistics |
format_facet |
Elektronische Aufsätze Aufsätze Elektronische Ressource |
format_main_str_mv |
Text Zeitschrift/Artikel |
carriertype_str_mv |
cr |
hierarchy_parent_title |
Language Development Research |
hierarchy_parent_id |
DOAJ090663721 |
hierarchy_top_title |
Language Development Research |
isfreeaccess_txt |
true |
familylinks_str_mv |
(DE-627)DOAJ090663721 |
title |
Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech |
ctrlnum |
(DE-627)DOAJ100834086 (DE-599)DOAJ78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 |
title_full |
Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech |
author_sort |
Alvin Wei Ming Tan |
journal |
Language Development Research |
journalStr |
Language Development Research |
callnumber-first-code |
P |
lang_code |
eng |
isOA_bool |
true |
recordtype |
marc |
publishDateSort |
2021 |
contenttype_str_mv |
txt |
author_browse |
Alvin Wei Ming Tan Kate Nation Nicola Dawson Nilanjana Banerji Yaling Hsiao |
container_volume |
1 |
class |
P1-1091 |
format_se |
Elektronische Aufsätze |
author-letter |
Alvin Wei Ming Tan |
doi_str_mv |
10.34842/5we1-yk94 |
author2-role |
verfasserin |
title_sort |
features of lexical richness in children's books: comparisons with child-directed speech |
callnumber |
P1-1091 |
title_auth |
Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech |
abstract |
Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book corpus from 160 texts commonly read to children aged 0-5 years (around 320,000 words). We first quantified how the language of children’s books differs from child-directed speech (compiled from 10 corpora in the CHILDES UK database, around 3.8 million words) on measures of lexical richness (diversity, density, sophistication), part of speech distributions, and structural properties. We also identified the words occurring in children’s books that are most uniquely representative of book language. We found that children’s book language is lexically denser, more lexically diverse, and comprises a larger proportion of rarer word types compared to child-directed speech. Nouns and adjectives are more common in book language whereas pronouns are more common in child-directed speech. Book words are more structurally complex in relation to both number of phonemes and morphological structure. They are also later acquired, more abstract, and more emotionally arousing than the words more common in child-directed speech. Written language provides unique linguistic input even in the pre-school years, well before children can read for themselves. |
abstractGer |
Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book corpus from 160 texts commonly read to children aged 0-5 years (around 320,000 words). We first quantified how the language of children’s books differs from child-directed speech (compiled from 10 corpora in the CHILDES UK database, around 3.8 million words) on measures of lexical richness (diversity, density, sophistication), part of speech distributions, and structural properties. We also identified the words occurring in children’s books that are most uniquely representative of book language. We found that children’s book language is lexically denser, more lexically diverse, and comprises a larger proportion of rarer word types compared to child-directed speech. Nouns and adjectives are more common in book language whereas pronouns are more common in child-directed speech. Book words are more structurally complex in relation to both number of phonemes and morphological structure. They are also later acquired, more abstract, and more emotionally arousing than the words more common in child-directed speech. Written language provides unique linguistic input even in the pre-school years, well before children can read for themselves. |
abstract_unstemmed |
Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book corpus from 160 texts commonly read to children aged 0-5 years (around 320,000 words). We first quantified how the language of children’s books differs from child-directed speech (compiled from 10 corpora in the CHILDES UK database, around 3.8 million words) on measures of lexical richness (diversity, density, sophistication), part of speech distributions, and structural properties. We also identified the words occurring in children’s books that are most uniquely representative of book language. We found that children’s book language is lexically denser, more lexically diverse, and comprises a larger proportion of rarer word types compared to child-directed speech. Nouns and adjectives are more common in book language whereas pronouns are more common in child-directed speech. Book words are more structurally complex in relation to both number of phonemes and morphological structure. They are also later acquired, more abstract, and more emotionally arousing than the words more common in child-directed speech. Written language provides unique linguistic input even in the pre-school years, well before children can read for themselves. |
collection_details |
GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_DOAJ |
container_issue |
1 |
title_short |
Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech |
url |
https://doi.org/10.34842/5we1-yk94 https://doaj.org/article/78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869 https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/id/77/ https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/77/galley/451/download/ https://doaj.org/toc/2771-7976 |
remote_bool |
true |
author2 |
Kate Nation Nicola Dawson Nilanjana Banerji Yaling Hsiao |
author2Str |
Kate Nation Nicola Dawson Nilanjana Banerji Yaling Hsiao |
ppnlink |
DOAJ090663721 |
callnumber-subject |
P - Philology and Linguistics |
mediatype_str_mv |
c |
isOA_txt |
true |
hochschulschrift_bool |
false |
doi_str |
10.34842/5we1-yk94 |
callnumber-a |
P1-1091 |
up_date |
2024-07-03T16:57:01.711Z |
_version_ |
1803577796026433537 |
fullrecord_marcxml |
<?xml version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8"?><collection xmlns="http://www.loc.gov/MARC21/slim"><record><leader>01000naa a22002652 4500</leader><controlfield tag="001">DOAJ100834086</controlfield><controlfield tag="003">DE-627</controlfield><controlfield tag="005">20240414130229.0</controlfield><controlfield tag="007">cr uuu---uuuuu</controlfield><controlfield tag="008">240414s2021 xx |||||o 00| ||eng c</controlfield><datafield tag="024" ind1="7" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">10.34842/5we1-yk94</subfield><subfield code="2">doi</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="035" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">(DE-627)DOAJ100834086</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="035" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">(DE-599)DOAJ78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="040" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">DE-627</subfield><subfield code="b">ger</subfield><subfield code="c">DE-627</subfield><subfield code="e">rakwb</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="041" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">eng</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="050" ind1=" " ind2="0"><subfield code="a">P1-1091</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="100" ind1="0" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Alvin Wei Ming Tan</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="245" ind1="1" ind2="0"><subfield code="a">Features of lexical richness in children's books: Comparisons with child-directed speech</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="264" ind1=" " ind2="1"><subfield code="c">2021</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="336" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Text</subfield><subfield code="b">txt</subfield><subfield code="2">rdacontent</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="337" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Computermedien</subfield><subfield code="b">c</subfield><subfield code="2">rdamedia</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="338" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Online-Ressource</subfield><subfield code="b">cr</subfield><subfield code="2">rdacarrier</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="520" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Access to children’s books via shared reading may be a particularly rich source of linguistic input in the early years. To understand how exposure to book language supports children’s learning, it is important to identify how book language differs to everyday conversation. We created a picture book corpus from 160 texts commonly read to children aged 0-5 years (around 320,000 words). We first quantified how the language of children’s books differs from child-directed speech (compiled from 10 corpora in the CHILDES UK database, around 3.8 million words) on measures of lexical richness (diversity, density, sophistication), part of speech distributions, and structural properties. We also identified the words occurring in children’s books that are most uniquely representative of book language. We found that children’s book language is lexically denser, more lexically diverse, and comprises a larger proportion of rarer word types compared to child-directed speech. Nouns and adjectives are more common in book language whereas pronouns are more common in child-directed speech. Book words are more structurally complex in relation to both number of phonemes and morphological structure. They are also later acquired, more abstract, and more emotionally arousing than the words more common in child-directed speech. Written language provides unique linguistic input even in the pre-school years, well before children can read for themselves.</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">lexical richness</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">book language</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">children</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">child-directed speech</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">language acquisition</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">literacy</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="653" ind1=" " ind2="0"><subfield code="a">Philology. Linguistics</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="700" ind1="0" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Kate Nation</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="700" ind1="0" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Nicola Dawson</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="700" ind1="0" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Nilanjana Banerji</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="700" ind1="0" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Yaling Hsiao</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="773" ind1="0" ind2="8"><subfield code="i">In</subfield><subfield code="t">Language Development Research</subfield><subfield code="d">Carnegie Mellon University Library Publishing Service, 2024</subfield><subfield code="g">1(2021), 1</subfield><subfield code="w">(DE-627)DOAJ090663721</subfield><subfield code="x">27717976</subfield><subfield code="7">nnns</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="773" ind1="1" ind2="8"><subfield code="g">volume:1</subfield><subfield code="g">year:2021</subfield><subfield code="g">number:1</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="0"><subfield code="u">https://doi.org/10.34842/5we1-yk94</subfield><subfield code="z">kostenfrei</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="0"><subfield code="u">https://doaj.org/article/78bbaf1158514689946fc6952dd46869</subfield><subfield code="z">kostenfrei</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="0"><subfield code="u">https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/id/77/</subfield><subfield code="z">kostenfrei</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="0"><subfield code="u">https://lps.library.cmu.edu/LDR/article/77/galley/451/download/</subfield><subfield code="z">kostenfrei</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="2"><subfield code="u">https://doaj.org/toc/2771-7976</subfield><subfield code="y">Journal toc</subfield><subfield code="z">kostenfrei</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_USEFLAG_A</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">SYSFLAG_A</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_DOAJ</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="951" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">AR</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="952" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="d">1</subfield><subfield code="j">2021</subfield><subfield code="e">1</subfield></datafield></record></collection>
|
score |
7.402011 |