Spelling errors in Spanish
Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Si...
Ausführliche Beschreibung
Autor*in: |
Valle-Arroyo, Francisco [verfasserIn] |
---|
Format: |
Artikel |
---|---|
Sprache: |
Englisch |
Erschienen: |
1990 |
---|
Schlagwörter: |
---|
Anmerkung: |
© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990 |
---|
Übergeordnetes Werk: |
Enthalten in: Reading and writing - Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1989, 2(1990), 1 vom: März, Seite 83-98 |
---|---|
Übergeordnetes Werk: |
volume:2 ; year:1990 ; number:1 ; month:03 ; pages:83-98 |
Links: |
---|
DOI / URN: |
10.1007/BF00383375 |
---|
Katalog-ID: |
OLC2070934705 |
---|
LEADER | 01000caa a22002652 4500 | ||
---|---|---|---|
001 | OLC2070934705 | ||
003 | DE-627 | ||
005 | 20230504022244.0 | ||
007 | tu | ||
008 | 200820s1990 xx ||||| 00| ||eng c | ||
024 | 7 | |a 10.1007/BF00383375 |2 doi | |
035 | |a (DE-627)OLC2070934705 | ||
035 | |a (DE-He213)BF00383375-p | ||
040 | |a DE-627 |b ger |c DE-627 |e rakwb | ||
041 | |a eng | ||
082 | 0 | 4 | |a 150 |a 400 |q VZ |
084 | |a 24,1 |a 5,3 |a 7,11 |2 ssgn | ||
084 | |a LING |q DE-30 |2 fid | ||
100 | 1 | |a Valle-Arroyo, Francisco |e verfasserin |4 aut | |
245 | 1 | 0 | |a Spelling errors in Spanish |
264 | 1 | |c 1990 | |
336 | |a Text |b txt |2 rdacontent | ||
337 | |a ohne Hilfsmittel zu benutzen |b n |2 rdamedia | ||
338 | |a Band |b nc |2 rdacarrier | ||
500 | |a © Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990 | ||
520 | |a Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Sixty children (20 second, 20 fourth and 20 eighth graders) were dictated 216 verbal stimuli. Half of these stimuli were words and the other half were matched nonwords. The dependent variable was the number of errors made. Words were controlled for frequency, length and regularity. Results show that second graders rely heavily on phonological mediation with:a significantly higher number of errors committed in words than in nonwords;length affects accuracy, with more errors produced on longer words (or nonwords); andregular words are spelled significantly better than irregular words. Eighth graders, on the other hand, show the complementary pattern of results and, consequently, seem to use a lexical strategy (via the graphemic output lexicon). The only deviant result in this group is a significant regularity effect, although the degree of this effect is smaller than in second graders. Fourth grade children show an intermediate pattern. A qualitative analysis (error types) also supports predictions made by the dual-route models. Some final considerations are proposed to explain the regularity effect in eighth graders and, based on the pattern of phonological errors, the possibility of a functional interdependence of both strategies of spelling. | ||
650 | 4 | |a Spelling models | |
650 | 4 | |a Spelling dual-route models | |
650 | 4 | |a Spelling errors | |
650 | 4 | |a Orthographic errors | |
650 | 4 | |a Phonological errors | |
773 | 0 | 8 | |i Enthalten in |t Reading and writing |d Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1989 |g 2(1990), 1 vom: März, Seite 83-98 |w (DE-627)182284557 |w (DE-600)1193661-7 |w (DE-576)515052175 |x 0922-4777 |7 nnns |
773 | 1 | 8 | |g volume:2 |g year:1990 |g number:1 |g month:03 |g pages:83-98 |
856 | 4 | 1 | |u https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00383375 |z lizenzpflichtig |3 Volltext |
912 | |a GBV_USEFLAG_A | ||
912 | |a SYSFLAG_A | ||
912 | |a GBV_OLC | ||
912 | |a FID-LING | ||
912 | |a SSG-OLC-BUB | ||
912 | |a SSG-OPC-BBI | ||
912 | |a GBV_ILN_40 | ||
912 | |a GBV_ILN_70 | ||
912 | |a GBV_ILN_4012 | ||
912 | |a GBV_ILN_4112 | ||
912 | |a GBV_ILN_4310 | ||
951 | |a AR | ||
952 | |d 2 |j 1990 |e 1 |c 03 |h 83-98 |
author_variant |
f v a fva |
---|---|
matchkey_str |
article:09224777:1990----::pligros |
hierarchy_sort_str |
1990 |
publishDate |
1990 |
allfields |
10.1007/BF00383375 doi (DE-627)OLC2070934705 (DE-He213)BF00383375-p DE-627 ger DE-627 rakwb eng 150 400 VZ 24,1 5,3 7,11 ssgn LING DE-30 fid Valle-Arroyo, Francisco verfasserin aut Spelling errors in Spanish 1990 Text txt rdacontent ohne Hilfsmittel zu benutzen n rdamedia Band nc rdacarrier © Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990 Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Sixty children (20 second, 20 fourth and 20 eighth graders) were dictated 216 verbal stimuli. Half of these stimuli were words and the other half were matched nonwords. The dependent variable was the number of errors made. Words were controlled for frequency, length and regularity. Results show that second graders rely heavily on phonological mediation with:a significantly higher number of errors committed in words than in nonwords;length affects accuracy, with more errors produced on longer words (or nonwords); andregular words are spelled significantly better than irregular words. Eighth graders, on the other hand, show the complementary pattern of results and, consequently, seem to use a lexical strategy (via the graphemic output lexicon). The only deviant result in this group is a significant regularity effect, although the degree of this effect is smaller than in second graders. Fourth grade children show an intermediate pattern. A qualitative analysis (error types) also supports predictions made by the dual-route models. Some final considerations are proposed to explain the regularity effect in eighth graders and, based on the pattern of phonological errors, the possibility of a functional interdependence of both strategies of spelling. Spelling models Spelling dual-route models Spelling errors Orthographic errors Phonological errors Enthalten in Reading and writing Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1989 2(1990), 1 vom: März, Seite 83-98 (DE-627)182284557 (DE-600)1193661-7 (DE-576)515052175 0922-4777 nnns volume:2 year:1990 number:1 month:03 pages:83-98 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00383375 lizenzpflichtig Volltext GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_OLC FID-LING SSG-OLC-BUB SSG-OPC-BBI GBV_ILN_40 GBV_ILN_70 GBV_ILN_4012 GBV_ILN_4112 GBV_ILN_4310 AR 2 1990 1 03 83-98 |
spelling |
10.1007/BF00383375 doi (DE-627)OLC2070934705 (DE-He213)BF00383375-p DE-627 ger DE-627 rakwb eng 150 400 VZ 24,1 5,3 7,11 ssgn LING DE-30 fid Valle-Arroyo, Francisco verfasserin aut Spelling errors in Spanish 1990 Text txt rdacontent ohne Hilfsmittel zu benutzen n rdamedia Band nc rdacarrier © Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990 Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Sixty children (20 second, 20 fourth and 20 eighth graders) were dictated 216 verbal stimuli. Half of these stimuli were words and the other half were matched nonwords. The dependent variable was the number of errors made. Words were controlled for frequency, length and regularity. Results show that second graders rely heavily on phonological mediation with:a significantly higher number of errors committed in words than in nonwords;length affects accuracy, with more errors produced on longer words (or nonwords); andregular words are spelled significantly better than irregular words. Eighth graders, on the other hand, show the complementary pattern of results and, consequently, seem to use a lexical strategy (via the graphemic output lexicon). The only deviant result in this group is a significant regularity effect, although the degree of this effect is smaller than in second graders. Fourth grade children show an intermediate pattern. A qualitative analysis (error types) also supports predictions made by the dual-route models. Some final considerations are proposed to explain the regularity effect in eighth graders and, based on the pattern of phonological errors, the possibility of a functional interdependence of both strategies of spelling. Spelling models Spelling dual-route models Spelling errors Orthographic errors Phonological errors Enthalten in Reading and writing Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1989 2(1990), 1 vom: März, Seite 83-98 (DE-627)182284557 (DE-600)1193661-7 (DE-576)515052175 0922-4777 nnns volume:2 year:1990 number:1 month:03 pages:83-98 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00383375 lizenzpflichtig Volltext GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_OLC FID-LING SSG-OLC-BUB SSG-OPC-BBI GBV_ILN_40 GBV_ILN_70 GBV_ILN_4012 GBV_ILN_4112 GBV_ILN_4310 AR 2 1990 1 03 83-98 |
allfields_unstemmed |
10.1007/BF00383375 doi (DE-627)OLC2070934705 (DE-He213)BF00383375-p DE-627 ger DE-627 rakwb eng 150 400 VZ 24,1 5,3 7,11 ssgn LING DE-30 fid Valle-Arroyo, Francisco verfasserin aut Spelling errors in Spanish 1990 Text txt rdacontent ohne Hilfsmittel zu benutzen n rdamedia Band nc rdacarrier © Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990 Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Sixty children (20 second, 20 fourth and 20 eighth graders) were dictated 216 verbal stimuli. Half of these stimuli were words and the other half were matched nonwords. The dependent variable was the number of errors made. Words were controlled for frequency, length and regularity. Results show that second graders rely heavily on phonological mediation with:a significantly higher number of errors committed in words than in nonwords;length affects accuracy, with more errors produced on longer words (or nonwords); andregular words are spelled significantly better than irregular words. Eighth graders, on the other hand, show the complementary pattern of results and, consequently, seem to use a lexical strategy (via the graphemic output lexicon). The only deviant result in this group is a significant regularity effect, although the degree of this effect is smaller than in second graders. Fourth grade children show an intermediate pattern. A qualitative analysis (error types) also supports predictions made by the dual-route models. Some final considerations are proposed to explain the regularity effect in eighth graders and, based on the pattern of phonological errors, the possibility of a functional interdependence of both strategies of spelling. Spelling models Spelling dual-route models Spelling errors Orthographic errors Phonological errors Enthalten in Reading and writing Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1989 2(1990), 1 vom: März, Seite 83-98 (DE-627)182284557 (DE-600)1193661-7 (DE-576)515052175 0922-4777 nnns volume:2 year:1990 number:1 month:03 pages:83-98 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00383375 lizenzpflichtig Volltext GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_OLC FID-LING SSG-OLC-BUB SSG-OPC-BBI GBV_ILN_40 GBV_ILN_70 GBV_ILN_4012 GBV_ILN_4112 GBV_ILN_4310 AR 2 1990 1 03 83-98 |
allfieldsGer |
10.1007/BF00383375 doi (DE-627)OLC2070934705 (DE-He213)BF00383375-p DE-627 ger DE-627 rakwb eng 150 400 VZ 24,1 5,3 7,11 ssgn LING DE-30 fid Valle-Arroyo, Francisco verfasserin aut Spelling errors in Spanish 1990 Text txt rdacontent ohne Hilfsmittel zu benutzen n rdamedia Band nc rdacarrier © Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990 Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Sixty children (20 second, 20 fourth and 20 eighth graders) were dictated 216 verbal stimuli. Half of these stimuli were words and the other half were matched nonwords. The dependent variable was the number of errors made. Words were controlled for frequency, length and regularity. Results show that second graders rely heavily on phonological mediation with:a significantly higher number of errors committed in words than in nonwords;length affects accuracy, with more errors produced on longer words (or nonwords); andregular words are spelled significantly better than irregular words. Eighth graders, on the other hand, show the complementary pattern of results and, consequently, seem to use a lexical strategy (via the graphemic output lexicon). The only deviant result in this group is a significant regularity effect, although the degree of this effect is smaller than in second graders. Fourth grade children show an intermediate pattern. A qualitative analysis (error types) also supports predictions made by the dual-route models. Some final considerations are proposed to explain the regularity effect in eighth graders and, based on the pattern of phonological errors, the possibility of a functional interdependence of both strategies of spelling. Spelling models Spelling dual-route models Spelling errors Orthographic errors Phonological errors Enthalten in Reading and writing Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1989 2(1990), 1 vom: März, Seite 83-98 (DE-627)182284557 (DE-600)1193661-7 (DE-576)515052175 0922-4777 nnns volume:2 year:1990 number:1 month:03 pages:83-98 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00383375 lizenzpflichtig Volltext GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_OLC FID-LING SSG-OLC-BUB SSG-OPC-BBI GBV_ILN_40 GBV_ILN_70 GBV_ILN_4012 GBV_ILN_4112 GBV_ILN_4310 AR 2 1990 1 03 83-98 |
allfieldsSound |
10.1007/BF00383375 doi (DE-627)OLC2070934705 (DE-He213)BF00383375-p DE-627 ger DE-627 rakwb eng 150 400 VZ 24,1 5,3 7,11 ssgn LING DE-30 fid Valle-Arroyo, Francisco verfasserin aut Spelling errors in Spanish 1990 Text txt rdacontent ohne Hilfsmittel zu benutzen n rdamedia Band nc rdacarrier © Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990 Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Sixty children (20 second, 20 fourth and 20 eighth graders) were dictated 216 verbal stimuli. Half of these stimuli were words and the other half were matched nonwords. The dependent variable was the number of errors made. Words were controlled for frequency, length and regularity. Results show that second graders rely heavily on phonological mediation with:a significantly higher number of errors committed in words than in nonwords;length affects accuracy, with more errors produced on longer words (or nonwords); andregular words are spelled significantly better than irregular words. Eighth graders, on the other hand, show the complementary pattern of results and, consequently, seem to use a lexical strategy (via the graphemic output lexicon). The only deviant result in this group is a significant regularity effect, although the degree of this effect is smaller than in second graders. Fourth grade children show an intermediate pattern. A qualitative analysis (error types) also supports predictions made by the dual-route models. Some final considerations are proposed to explain the regularity effect in eighth graders and, based on the pattern of phonological errors, the possibility of a functional interdependence of both strategies of spelling. Spelling models Spelling dual-route models Spelling errors Orthographic errors Phonological errors Enthalten in Reading and writing Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1989 2(1990), 1 vom: März, Seite 83-98 (DE-627)182284557 (DE-600)1193661-7 (DE-576)515052175 0922-4777 nnns volume:2 year:1990 number:1 month:03 pages:83-98 https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00383375 lizenzpflichtig Volltext GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_OLC FID-LING SSG-OLC-BUB SSG-OPC-BBI GBV_ILN_40 GBV_ILN_70 GBV_ILN_4012 GBV_ILN_4112 GBV_ILN_4310 AR 2 1990 1 03 83-98 |
language |
English |
source |
Enthalten in Reading and writing 2(1990), 1 vom: März, Seite 83-98 volume:2 year:1990 number:1 month:03 pages:83-98 |
sourceStr |
Enthalten in Reading and writing 2(1990), 1 vom: März, Seite 83-98 volume:2 year:1990 number:1 month:03 pages:83-98 |
format_phy_str_mv |
Article |
institution |
findex.gbv.de |
topic_facet |
Spelling models Spelling dual-route models Spelling errors Orthographic errors Phonological errors |
dewey-raw |
150 |
isfreeaccess_bool |
false |
container_title |
Reading and writing |
authorswithroles_txt_mv |
Valle-Arroyo, Francisco @@aut@@ |
publishDateDaySort_date |
1990-03-01T00:00:00Z |
hierarchy_top_id |
182284557 |
dewey-sort |
3150 |
id |
OLC2070934705 |
language_de |
englisch |
fullrecord |
<?xml version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8"?><collection xmlns="http://www.loc.gov/MARC21/slim"><record><leader>01000caa a22002652 4500</leader><controlfield tag="001">OLC2070934705</controlfield><controlfield tag="003">DE-627</controlfield><controlfield tag="005">20230504022244.0</controlfield><controlfield tag="007">tu</controlfield><controlfield tag="008">200820s1990 xx ||||| 00| ||eng c</controlfield><datafield tag="024" ind1="7" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">10.1007/BF00383375</subfield><subfield code="2">doi</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="035" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">(DE-627)OLC2070934705</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="035" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">(DE-He213)BF00383375-p</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="040" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">DE-627</subfield><subfield code="b">ger</subfield><subfield code="c">DE-627</subfield><subfield code="e">rakwb</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="041" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">eng</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="082" ind1="0" ind2="4"><subfield code="a">150</subfield><subfield code="a">400</subfield><subfield code="q">VZ</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="084" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">24,1</subfield><subfield code="a">5,3</subfield><subfield code="a">7,11</subfield><subfield code="2">ssgn</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="084" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">LING</subfield><subfield code="q">DE-30</subfield><subfield code="2">fid</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="100" ind1="1" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Valle-Arroyo, Francisco</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="245" ind1="1" ind2="0"><subfield code="a">Spelling errors in Spanish</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="264" ind1=" " ind2="1"><subfield code="c">1990</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="336" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Text</subfield><subfield code="b">txt</subfield><subfield code="2">rdacontent</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="337" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">ohne Hilfsmittel zu benutzen</subfield><subfield code="b">n</subfield><subfield code="2">rdamedia</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="338" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Band</subfield><subfield code="b">nc</subfield><subfield code="2">rdacarrier</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="500" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="520" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Sixty children (20 second, 20 fourth and 20 eighth graders) were dictated 216 verbal stimuli. Half of these stimuli were words and the other half were matched nonwords. The dependent variable was the number of errors made. Words were controlled for frequency, length and regularity. Results show that second graders rely heavily on phonological mediation with:a significantly higher number of errors committed in words than in nonwords;length affects accuracy, with more errors produced on longer words (or nonwords); andregular words are spelled significantly better than irregular words. Eighth graders, on the other hand, show the complementary pattern of results and, consequently, seem to use a lexical strategy (via the graphemic output lexicon). The only deviant result in this group is a significant regularity effect, although the degree of this effect is smaller than in second graders. Fourth grade children show an intermediate pattern. A qualitative analysis (error types) also supports predictions made by the dual-route models. Some final considerations are proposed to explain the regularity effect in eighth graders and, based on the pattern of phonological errors, the possibility of a functional interdependence of both strategies of spelling.</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">Spelling models</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">Spelling dual-route models</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">Spelling errors</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">Orthographic errors</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">Phonological errors</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="773" ind1="0" ind2="8"><subfield code="i">Enthalten in</subfield><subfield code="t">Reading and writing</subfield><subfield code="d">Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1989</subfield><subfield code="g">2(1990), 1 vom: März, Seite 83-98</subfield><subfield code="w">(DE-627)182284557</subfield><subfield code="w">(DE-600)1193661-7</subfield><subfield code="w">(DE-576)515052175</subfield><subfield code="x">0922-4777</subfield><subfield code="7">nnns</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="773" ind1="1" ind2="8"><subfield code="g">volume:2</subfield><subfield code="g">year:1990</subfield><subfield code="g">number:1</subfield><subfield code="g">month:03</subfield><subfield code="g">pages:83-98</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="1"><subfield code="u">https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00383375</subfield><subfield code="z">lizenzpflichtig</subfield><subfield code="3">Volltext</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_USEFLAG_A</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">SYSFLAG_A</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_OLC</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">FID-LING</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">SSG-OLC-BUB</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">SSG-OPC-BBI</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_ILN_40</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_ILN_70</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_ILN_4012</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_ILN_4112</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_ILN_4310</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="951" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">AR</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="952" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="d">2</subfield><subfield code="j">1990</subfield><subfield code="e">1</subfield><subfield code="c">03</subfield><subfield code="h">83-98</subfield></datafield></record></collection>
|
author |
Valle-Arroyo, Francisco |
spellingShingle |
Valle-Arroyo, Francisco ddc 150 ssgn 24,1 fid LING misc Spelling models misc Spelling dual-route models misc Spelling errors misc Orthographic errors misc Phonological errors Spelling errors in Spanish |
authorStr |
Valle-Arroyo, Francisco |
ppnlink_with_tag_str_mv |
@@773@@(DE-627)182284557 |
format |
Article |
dewey-ones |
150 - Psychology 400 - Language |
delete_txt_mv |
keep |
author_role |
aut |
collection |
OLC |
remote_str |
false |
illustrated |
Not Illustrated |
issn |
0922-4777 |
topic_title |
150 400 VZ 24,1 5,3 7,11 ssgn LING DE-30 fid Spelling errors in Spanish Spelling models Spelling dual-route models Spelling errors Orthographic errors Phonological errors |
topic |
ddc 150 ssgn 24,1 fid LING misc Spelling models misc Spelling dual-route models misc Spelling errors misc Orthographic errors misc Phonological errors |
topic_unstemmed |
ddc 150 ssgn 24,1 fid LING misc Spelling models misc Spelling dual-route models misc Spelling errors misc Orthographic errors misc Phonological errors |
topic_browse |
ddc 150 ssgn 24,1 fid LING misc Spelling models misc Spelling dual-route models misc Spelling errors misc Orthographic errors misc Phonological errors |
format_facet |
Aufsätze Gedruckte Aufsätze |
format_main_str_mv |
Text Zeitschrift/Artikel |
carriertype_str_mv |
nc |
hierarchy_parent_title |
Reading and writing |
hierarchy_parent_id |
182284557 |
dewey-tens |
150 - Psychology 400 - Language |
hierarchy_top_title |
Reading and writing |
isfreeaccess_txt |
false |
familylinks_str_mv |
(DE-627)182284557 (DE-600)1193661-7 (DE-576)515052175 |
title |
Spelling errors in Spanish |
ctrlnum |
(DE-627)OLC2070934705 (DE-He213)BF00383375-p |
title_full |
Spelling errors in Spanish |
author_sort |
Valle-Arroyo, Francisco |
journal |
Reading and writing |
journalStr |
Reading and writing |
lang_code |
eng |
isOA_bool |
false |
dewey-hundreds |
100 - Philosophy & psychology 400 - Language |
recordtype |
marc |
publishDateSort |
1990 |
contenttype_str_mv |
txt |
container_start_page |
83 |
author_browse |
Valle-Arroyo, Francisco |
container_volume |
2 |
class |
150 400 VZ 24,1 5,3 7,11 ssgn LING DE-30 fid |
format_se |
Aufsätze |
author-letter |
Valle-Arroyo, Francisco |
doi_str_mv |
10.1007/BF00383375 |
dewey-full |
150 400 |
title_sort |
spelling errors in spanish |
title_auth |
Spelling errors in Spanish |
abstract |
Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Sixty children (20 second, 20 fourth and 20 eighth graders) were dictated 216 verbal stimuli. Half of these stimuli were words and the other half were matched nonwords. The dependent variable was the number of errors made. Words were controlled for frequency, length and regularity. Results show that second graders rely heavily on phonological mediation with:a significantly higher number of errors committed in words than in nonwords;length affects accuracy, with more errors produced on longer words (or nonwords); andregular words are spelled significantly better than irregular words. Eighth graders, on the other hand, show the complementary pattern of results and, consequently, seem to use a lexical strategy (via the graphemic output lexicon). The only deviant result in this group is a significant regularity effect, although the degree of this effect is smaller than in second graders. Fourth grade children show an intermediate pattern. A qualitative analysis (error types) also supports predictions made by the dual-route models. Some final considerations are proposed to explain the regularity effect in eighth graders and, based on the pattern of phonological errors, the possibility of a functional interdependence of both strategies of spelling. © Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990 |
abstractGer |
Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Sixty children (20 second, 20 fourth and 20 eighth graders) were dictated 216 verbal stimuli. Half of these stimuli were words and the other half were matched nonwords. The dependent variable was the number of errors made. Words were controlled for frequency, length and regularity. Results show that second graders rely heavily on phonological mediation with:a significantly higher number of errors committed in words than in nonwords;length affects accuracy, with more errors produced on longer words (or nonwords); andregular words are spelled significantly better than irregular words. Eighth graders, on the other hand, show the complementary pattern of results and, consequently, seem to use a lexical strategy (via the graphemic output lexicon). The only deviant result in this group is a significant regularity effect, although the degree of this effect is smaller than in second graders. Fourth grade children show an intermediate pattern. A qualitative analysis (error types) also supports predictions made by the dual-route models. Some final considerations are proposed to explain the regularity effect in eighth graders and, based on the pattern of phonological errors, the possibility of a functional interdependence of both strategies of spelling. © Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990 |
abstract_unstemmed |
Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Sixty children (20 second, 20 fourth and 20 eighth graders) were dictated 216 verbal stimuli. Half of these stimuli were words and the other half were matched nonwords. The dependent variable was the number of errors made. Words were controlled for frequency, length and regularity. Results show that second graders rely heavily on phonological mediation with:a significantly higher number of errors committed in words than in nonwords;length affects accuracy, with more errors produced on longer words (or nonwords); andregular words are spelled significantly better than irregular words. Eighth graders, on the other hand, show the complementary pattern of results and, consequently, seem to use a lexical strategy (via the graphemic output lexicon). The only deviant result in this group is a significant regularity effect, although the degree of this effect is smaller than in second graders. Fourth grade children show an intermediate pattern. A qualitative analysis (error types) also supports predictions made by the dual-route models. Some final considerations are proposed to explain the regularity effect in eighth graders and, based on the pattern of phonological errors, the possibility of a functional interdependence of both strategies of spelling. © Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990 |
collection_details |
GBV_USEFLAG_A SYSFLAG_A GBV_OLC FID-LING SSG-OLC-BUB SSG-OPC-BBI GBV_ILN_40 GBV_ILN_70 GBV_ILN_4012 GBV_ILN_4112 GBV_ILN_4310 |
container_issue |
1 |
title_short |
Spelling errors in Spanish |
url |
https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00383375 |
remote_bool |
false |
ppnlink |
182284557 |
mediatype_str_mv |
n |
isOA_txt |
false |
hochschulschrift_bool |
false |
doi_str |
10.1007/BF00383375 |
up_date |
2024-07-04T02:36:29.317Z |
_version_ |
1803614252508905472 |
fullrecord_marcxml |
<?xml version="1.0" encoding="UTF-8"?><collection xmlns="http://www.loc.gov/MARC21/slim"><record><leader>01000caa a22002652 4500</leader><controlfield tag="001">OLC2070934705</controlfield><controlfield tag="003">DE-627</controlfield><controlfield tag="005">20230504022244.0</controlfield><controlfield tag="007">tu</controlfield><controlfield tag="008">200820s1990 xx ||||| 00| ||eng c</controlfield><datafield tag="024" ind1="7" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">10.1007/BF00383375</subfield><subfield code="2">doi</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="035" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">(DE-627)OLC2070934705</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="035" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">(DE-He213)BF00383375-p</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="040" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">DE-627</subfield><subfield code="b">ger</subfield><subfield code="c">DE-627</subfield><subfield code="e">rakwb</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="041" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">eng</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="082" ind1="0" ind2="4"><subfield code="a">150</subfield><subfield code="a">400</subfield><subfield code="q">VZ</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="084" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">24,1</subfield><subfield code="a">5,3</subfield><subfield code="a">7,11</subfield><subfield code="2">ssgn</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="084" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">LING</subfield><subfield code="q">DE-30</subfield><subfield code="2">fid</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="100" ind1="1" ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Valle-Arroyo, Francisco</subfield><subfield code="e">verfasserin</subfield><subfield code="4">aut</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="245" ind1="1" ind2="0"><subfield code="a">Spelling errors in Spanish</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="264" ind1=" " ind2="1"><subfield code="c">1990</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="336" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Text</subfield><subfield code="b">txt</subfield><subfield code="2">rdacontent</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="337" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">ohne Hilfsmittel zu benutzen</subfield><subfield code="b">n</subfield><subfield code="2">rdamedia</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="338" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Band</subfield><subfield code="b">nc</subfield><subfield code="2">rdacarrier</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="500" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">© Kluwer Academic Publishers 1990</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="520" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">Abstract The main purpose of this paper was to see whether dual-route models of spelling, developed within the context of opaque languages and supported mainly by the spelling patterns of acquired dysgraphics, could account for the misspellings of Spanish children of different educational levels. Sixty children (20 second, 20 fourth and 20 eighth graders) were dictated 216 verbal stimuli. Half of these stimuli were words and the other half were matched nonwords. The dependent variable was the number of errors made. Words were controlled for frequency, length and regularity. Results show that second graders rely heavily on phonological mediation with:a significantly higher number of errors committed in words than in nonwords;length affects accuracy, with more errors produced on longer words (or nonwords); andregular words are spelled significantly better than irregular words. Eighth graders, on the other hand, show the complementary pattern of results and, consequently, seem to use a lexical strategy (via the graphemic output lexicon). The only deviant result in this group is a significant regularity effect, although the degree of this effect is smaller than in second graders. Fourth grade children show an intermediate pattern. A qualitative analysis (error types) also supports predictions made by the dual-route models. Some final considerations are proposed to explain the regularity effect in eighth graders and, based on the pattern of phonological errors, the possibility of a functional interdependence of both strategies of spelling.</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">Spelling models</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">Spelling dual-route models</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">Spelling errors</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">Orthographic errors</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="650" ind1=" " ind2="4"><subfield code="a">Phonological errors</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="773" ind1="0" ind2="8"><subfield code="i">Enthalten in</subfield><subfield code="t">Reading and writing</subfield><subfield code="d">Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1989</subfield><subfield code="g">2(1990), 1 vom: März, Seite 83-98</subfield><subfield code="w">(DE-627)182284557</subfield><subfield code="w">(DE-600)1193661-7</subfield><subfield code="w">(DE-576)515052175</subfield><subfield code="x">0922-4777</subfield><subfield code="7">nnns</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="773" ind1="1" ind2="8"><subfield code="g">volume:2</subfield><subfield code="g">year:1990</subfield><subfield code="g">number:1</subfield><subfield code="g">month:03</subfield><subfield code="g">pages:83-98</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="856" ind1="4" ind2="1"><subfield code="u">https://doi.org/10.1007/BF00383375</subfield><subfield code="z">lizenzpflichtig</subfield><subfield code="3">Volltext</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_USEFLAG_A</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">SYSFLAG_A</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_OLC</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">FID-LING</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">SSG-OLC-BUB</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">SSG-OPC-BBI</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_ILN_40</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_ILN_70</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_ILN_4012</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_ILN_4112</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="912" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">GBV_ILN_4310</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="951" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="a">AR</subfield></datafield><datafield tag="952" ind1=" " ind2=" "><subfield code="d">2</subfield><subfield code="j">1990</subfield><subfield code="e">1</subfield><subfield code="c">03</subfield><subfield code="h">83-98</subfield></datafield></record></collection>
|
score |
7.400079 |